Book
Review by Bob Seitz
Genius
The Natural History
of Creativity
written by
Dr. Hans Eysenck
Until his
death in 199, Dr. Hans Eysenck was one of the world's leading
experts on intelligence testing. In 1995, he wrote this analysis
of the wellsprings of genius, published by the Press Syndicate of
the University of Cambridge.
Dr. Eysenck first
mentions Sir Francis Galton's categorizations of eminence.
Galton's threshold level of eminence, Class F, occurs about once
in every 4,300 adults; his Class G, has a frequency of 1 in
79,000; and his Class X, consists of 1 in 1,000,000 individuals.
It's a startling fact that not a single one of Terman's 1,528
gifted children out of 250,000 California schoolchildren, made it
into Galton's Class F (1 in 4,300)! Galton argued that genius is
a product of (1) intelligence/special abilities, (2)
persistence/hard work, and (3) striving. (Modern psychologists,
including Dr. Eysenck, might lean more toward
intelligence/special aptitudes, striving, and originality).
Creative output and eminence follows a Pareto-like J-curve such
as . Here, C is a scaling constant, and "a"
may be adjusted to fit the experimental data. An outgrowth of
Pareto's formula, Price' Law, states that if there are
"n" contributors in a field, half of the contributions
will come from
contributors. For example, if there are
1,024 contributors, half the output will come from 32 of them.
Other factors that enter into the title of "genius" are
luck, and the question of who writes the history books. As Sir
William Osler put it, "In science, the credit goes to the
man who convinces the world, not to the man to whom the idea
first occurs!"
Dr. Eysenck
concludes that the typical genius has "an IQ some three to
four standard deviations above the mean." "In the long
run such precision does not matter very much; the main point is
that high IQ is one of the features of genius, and apparently a
universal, and hence probably a necessary, one."
Another telling
tale is the outcome of children with extremely high IQ's. Many,
but not all of them end up in professional or academic careers.
Dr. Eysenck mentions several British children who didn't. One was
a boy who read fluently at three, who became a university
dropout, spent two years on the dole, and is now a low-paid
office manager. Another fellow who had an IQ of 170 at 9 is a van
driver. One musical prodigy with an IQ of 169, at 29 is running a
band. She lacked ambition. Another female prodigy is now a
full-time mother and is very content with her life. Not one of
Dr. Leta Hollingworth's children above 180 "is known to have
developed to a 'genius' level or anything approaching it".
In my own personal experience, one friend, with a childhood IQ of
169, became a Ph. D. psychologist, earning a J. D. at night.
Another friend, who scored 1600 on the Graduate Record Exam in
the 50's when this maight have been the highest score in the
country, got his Ph. D. at Harvard and then spent a successful,
though quiet, career in a university physics department. (One of
his departmental chairmen once told him that he wasn't ruthless
enough.) Another individual, who rivalled William Sidis in early
precocity, taught physiology as an M. D. in the Harvard Medical
School.
Other studies
have identified drive and originality as playing a more important
role than native ability in the attainment of eminence, whether
in terms of IQ or specialized talents, although, as Dr. Eysenck
has noted, a high level of native ability seems to be a
prerequisite. . However, Dr. Eysenck reviews Roe's 1953 IQ
measurements of eminent scientists one level below Nobel
Laureates. Their median IQ was 166, even though some of them
reached the 177 ceiling of the test. Their median spatial IQ was
137, although it was felt that this would have been higher had
they been younger. Their median mathematical IQ was 154, ranging
from 128 to 194. They typically worked 70-hour weeks.
Dr. John Watson,
of Crick and Watson fame, recently observed that at the time that
he and Dr. Crick were working on the double-helix problem, there
were other brilliant researchers, including Dr. Linus Pauling,
who were also closing in on the solution. However, these other
researchers were highly competitive and unwilling to share their
thoughts with others, and it was this ability to work together
with other researchers that led Crick and Watson to the prize.
Dr. Eysenck also
observes that in certain fields, such as literature and art, an
unhappy childhood and/orpsychopathology seems to be a
contributing factor, perhaps permitting the author/artist to
better relate to the human condition.
In conclusion,
Dr. Eysenck concludes that intelligence is a necessary but not a
sufficient condition for genius. Focussed hard work, persistence,
originality, luck, and opportunity also play decisive
roles. He also suggests that the game may have changed somewhat
from Einstein's day because:
(1) there is so much to learn in any
branch of knowledge;
(2) there are so many more players;
and
(3) the fruit that hangs lowest on
the tree has already been picked.