Sumerian Grammar
by Patrick C. Ryan
(1/13/2008)
This grammar is based on the hypothesis that IE and Sumerian are both descended from a common ancestor, which, I term the Proto-Language — from the form into which it developed between 55-60K BPE.
The date is based on the estimates of Cavalli-Sforza for the separation of the peoples of Asia and Europe (The Great Human Diasporas, p. 123) from the "main" branch of the people speaking the Proto-Language.
During this phase of development, the Proto-Language was passing out of a class-type morphology into an ergative-type morphology (G. A. Klimov).
The word order of Sumerian is — like Basque ( Trask 1997:109) — consistently SOV , what we would expect from any language that preserves early syntax. Although "modifiers overwhelmingly precede their heads" in Basque (genitives and relative clauses; Trask 1997:122) — a further correlate of SOV typology, in Sumerian, genitives and relative clauses follow their referents; however, in Basque, "lexical adjectives follow the nouns they modify (Trask 1997:122)" just as in Sumerian, in which an adjective "stands directly after the noun which it qualifies ( Thomsen 1984:64)".
This discrepancy can be resolved when we realize that a number of Basque adjectives like ilun, "dark", also function as nouns: "darkness", which has led "a number of vasconists to suspect that, at some early stage of the language, there was no distinction between adjectives and nouns (Trask 1997:210)", which Trask admits as a possibility if "at a very remote period". This is certainly the case in Sumerian in which "Adjectives do not differ morphologically from nominal or verbal stems and there are no morphological means to derive adjectives from other stems. An adjectival stem is primarily characterized by its syntactic use . . . (Thomsen 1984:64)".
In certain words, an earlier word order of Adjective/Genitive+Noun seems
to be preserved in Sumerian, at least, graphically. The name of the moon-god,
Zu-en, 'lord of wisdom' or 'wise lord', is written zu-en. This is the expected
word order in a conforming SOV language. I interpret this to mean that when the
Sumerians invaded southern Mesopotamia, Sumerian was
Adjective/Genitive+Noun, and that a loss of this
original syntactic feature is due to contact with the people who were autochthonous in the
region, presumably Semitic speakers who preferred the word order of
Noun+Adjective/Genitive. Further support for the idea of an earlier Sumerian
word order of Adjective/Genitive/Relative-Clause+Noun comes from
Abû Salâbîkh (circa 2600 BP), where a passage from a
work known as
'Instructions of Šuruppak to his son Ziusudra', reads:
What is enormously exciting about Sumerian is that (unlike Basque and Japanese) it separated from the main branch of the Proto-Language after the stage of development (Pontic) in which the oldest semantic contrasts of CE / CA / CO were replaced by CyV, C(-)V, and CwV, the superscripts indicating semi-consonantal glides or no glide — in keeping with the pattern observed in other Caucasian languages, Sumerian lost the superfluous V before contact with Semitic. The contact with Semitic caused Sumerian C + glide to be replaced with vowels (Cy became Ci; Cw became Cu; while C- became Ca .
Therefore, in open syllables ( in the absence of a following /j/ or /w/), Sumerian
preserves a record of and Basque preserves the original vowel quality of the
Proto-Language antecedents intact.
In the Table of Correspondence found below after the grammar, the column entitled PROTO-LANGUAGE shows the earliest syllables before vocalic contrasts were replaced by a contrast of glides and no glide (during the Pontic stage: 60-40K BPE).
The interpretation of the Sumerian evidence has unique problemsa. Among which, most of the signs have multiple phonological values; and I have prepared a small series of essays which will explain the significance of these variations.
Current Sumerological practice is to indicate the commonest occurring word of any given form
with no diacritical (e.g. mu [**myu], 'name'), the next
commonest homophonous
sign with an acute accent (e.g. mú [**mü2], 'kindle'),
and the third
commonest with
a grave accent (e.g. mù [**mU3], 'grind'); and the
fourth
commonest, with a
subscript (e.g. mu4, 'clothe' [which may actually be, correctly,
mur10]).
I will indicate the acute accent by the subscript 2, and the grave
accent by
the subscript 3.
The Proto-Languageb used tone (rising and falling)
to indicate the beginning and
end of a statement. If the extraordinarily numerous (twelve mu's,
e.g.) supposedly homophonous words of Sumerian were not distinguished
by tone, for which there is not the slightest evidence, or in some other way, it would have been a
most untypical and burdensomely ambiguous language. What is likeliest, is that Sumerian
distinguished "homophones" by differences in vowel quality that the Akkadians were unable to
maintain after Sumerian ceased to be spoken. The Akkadians were enabled to maintain
Sumerian as a literary language because the "homophones" were not written identically, and the
different cuneiform signs employed usually predicted the meaning as, in my opinion, they
formerly indicated the vowel quality much more precisely.
In addition, modern Sumerian scholars have adopted the highly questionable idea that final
consonants — as in mu4(r10), 'clothe', above —
were dropped in certain positions or employments3,
compounding the problem of homophonous ambiguity for spoken (but not
necessarily written4) Sumerian.
Sumerian was not a language that needed greater homophony. In spite of that, however, most
Sumerologists believe that "Final consonants are
often omitted in the writing"5, and, presumably in the pronunciation as
well. We shall see, in the appropriate places, that this belief is completely unwarranted; and, it
is to Thomsen's credit, that she seems to be acknowledging this by writing: "Because of the uncertainties concerning the actual
pronunciation I have, as a principle, rendered the stems with the final consonant:
dug4 (not du11), zid (not zi),
níg [nig2] (not nì
[ni3])"6.
|
|
|
|
|
'at there' |
'on there' |
'from there' |
'to there, thither' |
'nearby' |
|
|
|
|
|
|
non-concomitant time (near future or near past) 'then' |
'nearby' |
proximative, "ergative", "(locative-) terminative" |
'that (nearby)' |
'immediate presence' |
|
|
|
|
|
|
immediate present 'now' |
'here' |
superessive, 'on' "locative" "genitive" |
'this' |
PL MORPHOLOGICAL ELEMENTS IN SUMERIAN
(not included under lexical headings)to investigate these phonological correspondences in detail, see the
TABLE OF PL / IE / SUMERIAN CORRESPONDENCES
NOTATIONAL CONVENTIONS
for modifications of the vowels and consonants in combination, see the
Table of Modifications
In order for readers to judge the semantic plausibility of the analysis of
Proto-Language (PL) compounds suggested here, I
am including access to a table of
Proto-Language monosyllables and the meanings I have
provisionally assigned.
Most assignments can be exhaustively supported by data from actually attested forms but a
few animates are very doubtful; and this list does not represent the "final" solution of these
questions, which will only be approached when other scholars assist in refining it.
Patrick C. Ryan
Summer 1998
the latest revision of this document can be found at
HTTP://WWW.GEOCITIES.COM/Athens/Forum/2803/SumerianGrammar.htm
Patrick C. Ryan * 9115 West 34th Street - Little Rock, AR
72204-4441 * (501)227-9947
PROTO-LANGUAGE@email.msn.com
a. There is hardly a linguistic field which so clearly shows the longterm effects of
incompetent scholarship as Sumerology, with a few notable exceptions like the
work of Thorkild Jacobsen. The most bizarre proposals and nonsensical translations of Sumerian
texts have been put forward but the clubby atmosphere of most fellow
Sumerologists prohibits even the mildest justifiable criticism. As in some other academic
disciplines, affirmative action has superseded demonstrated ability as the test for appointment;
and talentless "scholars" with the right quota credentials are recruited to join the megalomaniacs
like Samuel Noah Kramer who have already contaminated the field. The less talent, the more
arrogance; and for a demonstration of this, Gonzalo Rubio's webpage is ideal.
Here, a man who has published nothing of importance cavalierly dismisses the efforts of two
other individuals who have recently produced Sumerian grammars, however good they might or
might not be, ". . . because their author(sic!) is(sic!) not a Sumerologist(sic!) and, therefore,
they(sic!) offer a rather superficial and frequently misleading perspective(sic!)". Here is a
wannabe "Sumerologist" who cannot even write properly grammatical and punctuated English
presuming to condemn without discussion the scholarly work of others solely and sufficiently for
him because they are not members of his exclusive but tainted cabal. An unfavorable comment
from the ilk of Sr. Rubio is an accolade of high honor indeed; and anyone who
contemplates studying at Johns Hopkins should be aware of the level of competence demanded
of instructors at that institution.
b. I am aware that some readers, who may entertain the possibility of a Pontic-Nostratic
connection between Indo-European, Afrasian, and Sumerian, will not be able to accept the
possibility of a reconstruction of a language as early as the Proto-Language. To
those readers, may I suggest that the Proto-Language reconstructions and
interpretations be merely regarded as an expression of an arbitrary system of notation that allows
for the regular relationships of correspondence between Indo-European, Afrasian, and
Sumerian?
1. Thomsen
1984, pp. 21.
2. Thomsen
1984, pp. 39-40.
3. Thomsen
1984, p. 42.
4. Although two phonological forms for "enter" are postulated, ku4
and kur9, both are written with the same sign: J. 99.
5. Thomsen
1984, p. 42.
6. Thomsen
1984, p. 42.
7. Jaritz 1967, Sign
#594, the sign with which ga is written, has the recorded Akkadian reading gu(-
u), which would be the anticipated Sumerian reflex of PL
QO, 'attach(ed), also', which we believe is the basis for this element as evidenced in other
languages.
8. Jaritz Sign #805, with which u3 is written, is graphically
composed of the signs for 'eye' and 'cord', suggesting a basal meaning like 'the eye is drawn to'.
One of its meanings is 'cry of pain', **wê, which I believe represents the Sumerian
reflex of PL F[H]A-¿E, 'wail-ing', which is also seen in IE
*wai, 'interjection "woe"'; and AA **w-y as exemplified by Arabic
wai(H/ha), 'interjection of pity or threat'. In addition, Jaritz Combination
#3058, u3.di, is glossed as Akkadian kûru, 'woe, trouble,
misery'; I believe this correlates with IE **waid- in Middle Irish fa:ed,
fo:id, 'cry', and Latvian vai[~]di, '(pl.) cry of distress, crying, trouble'.
9. If my derivation for ê from **(?)aj is correct, i.e. from
PL ?A-¿E, it is legitimate to ask what would have been the
earliest and most appropriate sign to represent it. I believe that question can be answered.
Jaritz Sign #599 depicts a wall-mat for a house, and means 'house'; it has the reading
?e3 however is usually rendered as e2, showing that
the glottal stop (/?/) was lost over time. This may represent PL
?A-¿E, 'grass-like (thing)' or PL ?A-¿E,
'family-like thing' — either of which could lead to 'house'.
10. Since the formant for the allative is, according to my view
11. According again to G. B. Gragg, as summarized by Thomsen on p. 149-50: "The subject
mark -e- in preradical position has changed to -a- because of the preceding -da-; the second -a-
probably denotes some kind of transition between the pronominal prefix and the initial vowel of
the verb, cf. Gragg SDI p. 85."
12. In my opinion, one of the earliest terms for 'possession' is based on
The comments relating Sumerian grammatical forms to prototypes in the Proto-
Language will be highlighted in green so that they may
passed over by those whose interest is solely in Sumerian grammar.
A better explanation of the
phenomenon that final consonants are variationally resumed as an initial onset in a following
syllable that begins with a vowel is that the notation records a shift in stress-accent.
Interestingly, another word, derived from PL FA-¿E, 'leaf-like=fragile', seen
in IE *wa/a:i-, 'weak, miserable', is also present among the meanings listed for
Sumerian u3 but assigned to the reading si5
(cf. IE *sa:i-, 'pain, sickness, damage'; here we have an alternate reading
based on semantic similarity).
The conclusions I draw from these considerations are that Sumerian u3
originally represented **wê, and that the basal meaning of the sign originated with this
meaning.
Presumably, at the time that writing is first recorded, the glottal stop had been lost except after a
vowel, so that initial ?ê-, the non-concomitant verbal prefix ('then, thereupon'), and
-?ê, the demonstrative ('this nearby'), at least after a verb ending in a consonant, were
pronounced ê, which carried adverbial -?ê- ('there nearby') with them. When
Sign #599 acquired a commoner reading of **bêd for 'tent' (PL
P[?]A-¿E-T[?]O), the way was clear for a sign which consistently read
ê to represent earlier ?ê
This sign was Jaritz Sign #574, which depicts a canal, and means 'canal' or 'water'. This
derives from PL HHA-¿E, 'water-like', which probably went
through the stage of /**he:/ before becoming /e:/, i.e.
ê, presently transcribed e.
It is possible that i/î might represent a later purely phonological development from
*(?)aj, perhaps in a dialect other than that which had (?)ê but what I consider to
be more likely is that /*?i/ is from PL ?E, 'that yonder, then, over there',
the base for which is seen in Jaritz Sign #15, i5, 'tooth'. When
Sign #15 began to be read more frequently as ka, another sign that preferred
the reading of /i/i:/ was substituted for it.
Now i3, Jaritz Sign #456, the primary meaning of which appears
to be an 'oilpress', I connect with PL ¿O, 'squeeze'. Although
*iu is not an acknowledged reading of Sign #456, Akkadian records a reading
of ia?u, which I believe should be corrected to *iu, the anticipated Sumerian
result of ¿O. In its later manifestation, i3 will almost
certainly have represented î(3).
On the other hand, i is Jaritz Sign #270; it has the primary meaning of 'five',
which I connect with PL ¿A, 'many', which would have had the
anticipated Sumerian reflex of î/ia; I believe that the employment of this sign is purely
phonological.
It is highly probable that the earlier reading of Jaritz Sign #456 was *iu and the
earlier reading of Jaritz Sign #270 was *ia; it was only after both had been
simplified phonologically to /i:/ that they become substitutes for earlier
/?i/
A further correlate of this is that the time of the first use of this formant, at least many Sumerian
nouns retained a final vowel so that it was regularized as -e rather than -i.
This appears to be the case with the imperfective aspect of verbs as well. The ending -
e(n), which is termed the "present-future" by Sumerologists, is, we shall see, a result of
a + î, of which the î is the phonological reflex to PL
-¿E.