An Institute of Noetic Sciences Book
Page 60: Chapter 3
UNCONSCIOUS BELIEVING, CHOOSING, AND KNOWING
Why aren't there Beethovens, Gandhis, or Einsteins in everybody's family?
If everyone possesses an inate capacity for breakthrough to higher kinds
of creativity - what is it, exactly, that hides the key and holds most
of us back from discovering how to use these talents?
Among the deep unconscious beliefs everybody holds are beliefs about
human potentialities and limitations - one's own, and other people's.
These limits tend to be confirmed by experience, not because they are true
but because they are believed. In the familiar phenomena used in
demonstrations of hypnosis, a hypnotized person told to perceive a "solid
wall" where there is none can "perceive" the experience as so real that
capillaries burst and the hand becomes physically bruised from "striking"
the wall.
On the other hand, a different kind of suggestion may lead to the body
being able to perform feats it could not otherwise do - form a rigid bridge
between two chairs or lift a heavy object.
The currently acceptable models of human capacities, and people's beliefs
about the limits of those capacities, as viewed by our society and "approved"
by science, tend to be based on a number of implicit premises that have
gone unchallenged until recently. Following are examples:
1. "Mind is a function of the physical components of the brain, and only a function of the brain. Scientific understanding of human behavior ultimately can be framed in terms of physical processes in the brain."
2. "The only way people can acquire knowledge is, in the last analysis, through the physical senses."
3. "All qualitative properties are ultimately reducible to quantitative ones; in other words, color can be reduced to a relationship between wavelengths and events in the brain, hate and love and consciousness will one day be reduced to a chemical reaction of glandular secretions, etc. What we know as consciousness, or awareness of our thoughts and feelings, is really only a side effect of these physical and biochemical processes occurring in the brain."
4. "There is a clear distinction between the objective world, which is perceivable by anyone, and subjective experience, which is perceived by the individual alone in the privacy of his or her own mind.
5. "The concept of the 'free person' is a prescientific explanation for behavior. Behavior is determined by forces impinging upon individuals from the environment, in interaction with internal tensions characteristic of the human biological organism. From the standpoint of behavioral science, psychic freedom and free choice are mere illusions. 'Freedom' is behavior for which scientists have not yet found the cause."
6. "The average person possesses little or no genius or talent, and their capacity for inspiration is limited intellectually to the extent of whatever IQ they were fortunate (or unfortunate) enough to be born with."
7. "Belief in extraordinary psychic talents, such as ESP, out-ofbody experiences, etc. are superstitious holdovers from a less sophisticated era, and do not exist at all except as hallucinations, delusion, or wishful thinking."
Each of these statements seems reasonable enough and compatible with
science at its best - yet the evidence that we are examining in this book
will challenge all seven of these statements. Many of you may not
even look upon these statements as "beliefs," but consider them to be "facts"
- in the same way that several decades aago you might have argued that it
was a "fact" and not a "belief" that something could not be both a wave
and a particle at the same time.
History has shown, time and again, that popular opinion about human
limits can change, and that human limits themselves can change drastically.
In Europe, for thousands of years, there was no "fact" considered more
well established than the innate inability of peasants to learn how to
read. In the fifteenth century, the printing press was invented,
and the literate population jumped from a miniscule elite to a significant
portion of the general population. In the time of Samuel Johnson
the simple ability to perform sums in arithmetic was considered to be something
of an esoteric art. johnson's biographer, Boswell, noted how hard it was
for Johnson, surely one of the most educated men of his era, to teach himself
this arcane art.
Our lives and our behaviors are much more profoundly affected by the
beliefs we hold unconsciously than by the beliefs we hold consciously.
It is apparent that to reach the breakthrough state we must make a fundamental
shift in consciously and unconsciously held beliefs we all hold about our
own limitations. Moreover, it appears that there are ways in which
unconscious beliefs can be deliberately reprogrammed. And knowing
about the possibility of reshaping one's beliefs is the necessary prelude
to doing so.
The influence of beliefs begins with perception, the main tool we have
to judge external reality. Of all the stimuli that impinge on the
sensory receptors of sight, hearing, touch, smell and taste, only a small
portion ever reach our conscious experience awareness. Some "inner
observer" or mechanism unconsciously selects which inputs should reach
awareness and shunts the rest to some other part of the mind. This
selection is influenced by people's expectations, particularly as they
are conditioned by beliefs arising from previous experience or learning,
and by what people want or believe they need to perceive - or need not
to perceive.
The effect of beliefs and attitudes on visual perception even appears
to extend to the pupil of the eye, which tends to dilate (and thus increase
perception of light) when we are presented with something we want to see,
and contract (thus decreasing visual input) when there is something before
us we want not to see. Similar effects seem to hold for the other
senses as well.
Our processes of interpretation - the ways we organize sensory experience
and the methods we learn by which we assign meaning to what we perceive
- we also influenced by beliefs. FFor example, in an otherwise darkened
room, when an oversized playing card is displayed considerably further
away than an undersized football, both are seen as normal-sized, with the
playing card appearing closer and the football further away. In another
well-known experiment, rapidly exposed playing cards with reversed color-suit
combinations (black hearts, red spades) were often perceived as the subject
would normally expect to see them (red hearts, black spades).'
One of the most often cited examples of seeing what we expect to see
is the finding that poor children see coins as larger than do the children
of wealthy parents. Hungry subjects see more food objects in vague
pictures (so vague, in fact, that they are actually no more than smudges)
than do well-fed subjects, etc.
One of the most powerful formers of beliefs is the surrounding culture.
People who grow up in different cultures literally perceive the world in
different ways. The 1898 Cambridge anthropological expedition to
Torres Straits found that the natives were not fooled by optical illusions
that uniformly deceived Europeans. Malinowski observed that the Trobriand
Islanders, who believed that all characteristics are inherited from the
father, regularly failed to see resemblances of the child to the mother's
side of the family.
Extensive clinical and experimental research and anthropological observations
have provided further support for the hypothesis that to some degree we
only see what our culture tells us we can see, only know what our society
tells us we can know.
The findings from studies of hypnosis are particularly startling.
Through hypnotic suggestion we may see what no one else sees, or fail to
see what everyone else can see clearly; we may experience limitations where
there are none, or display extraordinary strength or mental facility.
The implication is especially jarring when we recognize- the similarity
between the "state of suggestibility" characteristic of hypnosis and the
power of the suggestions given by one's culture. We do not know the
extent to which we are all hypnotized to perceive the particular version
of reality that is communicated by our culture.
This cultural consensus changes over time, in both conscious and unconscious
aspects, in a variety of ways. Some beliefs, close to the "surface,"
but nevertheless connected to our deeper feelings about ourselves, can
be modified through the historical processes of science and education.
In 1492, the educated majority "knew" that the Earth is both flat and the
center of the universe. When sailors looked at the horizon back then,
they saw a place where they might very well fall off the Earth. A
few people like Columbus suspected that this knowledge was not factual,
but a matter of incorrect belief.
Even more deeply held and less consciously sensed beliefs about one's
most basic identity as a human being, and sense of one's relationship to
the rest of the universe, may be formed early in life and remain essentially
unchanged throughout life; if they are altered, it is likely to be in the
context of a life trauma of major proportions. "Enlightenment," 4
i satori...... samadhi," "being born again" are among the many names people
have given to those singular experiences powerful enough to reconfigure
the deepest unconscious beliefs.
In fact, we not only believe unconsciously but we also choose unconsciously.
One way we choose unconsciously is to protect ourselves by blocking from
our awareness information or experience that contradicts our unconscious
belief system (in what psychotherapists call "denial" - not seeing what's
there) or that implies any need for change in those beliefs (what psychotherapists
term "resistance"). According to this hypothesis, if we encounter
knowledge that would change our personality (an essential belief system),
the internal censor, operating deep in the unconscious, treats such knowledge
as a threat and attempts to ignore it.
Psychologist Abraham Maslow pointed out that we are all ambivalent
when it comes to knowing about ourselves; we want to know (consciously)
and yet we also go to great lengths (unconsciously) to prevent ourselves
from knowing. He identified this taboo against some kinds of inner
knowledge as "the need to know and the fear of knowing." As Maslow has
said, we fear to know the fearsome and unsavory aspects of ourselves (what
Jung aptly named "the shadow"), but we fear even more to know "the godlike
in ourselves."
We are ambivalent about our desire to know ourselves. We will
resist that knowledge which we most deeply desire. We think we want
to see reality as it is, to see it truthfully. But the illusions
we harbor are part of an unconsciously held belief system. Any attack
on those illusions is perceived (unconsciously) as a threat. Thus,
an effort to dispel illusion, although ultimately beneficial, may nevertheless
generate resistance. We may truly desire to discover and actualize
our highest capabilities. Yet to the extent that removing illusion
is essential to that discovery, we will resist that which we truly desire.
We may, in fact, resist it by using the most convincing "scientific" arguments
and concepts.
Part of the reason for this is that in our culture we have been systematically
taught not to trust our own minds. The Darwinian theory of evolution
was a product of the Victorian age, and even though our understanding of
evolutionary process has become much more sophisticated since then, the
idea still persists in our culture that there is only a "thin veneer of
civilization" between our conscious, socialized, ego-minds and the seething
subconscious memories of "nature red in tooth and claw" that lurk, barely
controlled, as animal urges
just beneath the surface.
Freud's concept of unconscious sexual and aggressive forces at odds
with the rules of civilized life added sinister and emotionally laden connotations
to the Victorian interpretation of evolution, as did tales of people who
"went off the deep end" because of their foolhardy insistence on exploring
the depths of their own minds. Better not to look in there, or if
you must, be sure to have a psychiatrist present to watch after you, because
what you find might drive you crazy. Of course, if one deeply believes
that the unconscious realms are to be generally feared and distrusted,
unpleasant experiences undoubtedly await one.
But surely there is something very strange
if not almost insane about the idea that one can't trust one's own mind.
We go to sleep every night, and part of us remembers how to breathe.
Every time we take a step, we throw our leg into space and fall forward,
trusting that our foot will land in just the right way to prevent us from
pitching onto our face. Learning how to walk in the first place was
one part learning how to master the movements and nine parts learning how
to trust the self to do it.
When you think about it, many of the things we do in a normal day involve
an enormous amount of trust in the capabilities of our minds. We
put ourselves in control of a vehicle weighing thousands of pounds and
navigate it through rush-hour traffic, all the while trusting our ability
to perceive accurately and make split-second, life-or-death decisions.
This cultural predisposition toward mistrust of our unconscious minds
(while relying on them at the same time) means that any serious attempt
at self-revelation will arouse strong and subtle resistance. Even
reading the relatively innocuous paragraphs above may arouse objections
that one can recognize as resistance by the intensity of associated feeling:
"People do 'go off the deep end'!" "Society would fall apart if we didn't
rationally control our unconscious minds!" And so on.
THE TABOO AGAINST INNER KNOWING
Science is a method for gathering and validating knowledge, a method
that has extraordinary credibility because of the technological marvels
it has wrought. But science, in the sense it is used when we judge
something to be "not scientific," is also an ideology - an institutionalized
belief system - and like any other belief system, science tends to have
its dogmas and its taboos. One of these taboos is very closely related
to the kind of resistance toward knowing about ourselves that we exhibit
as individuals.
The particular kind of cultural trance induced on inhabitants of the
Western world in the twentieth century involves the disguised enshrinement
within the institution of science of a long-standing and powerful taboo
against inner knowledge of an unauthorized kind. Modern science developed
in an industrializing society that put high value on those kinds of knowledge
that would contribute to the generation of mechanical technologies.
This emphasis led to a tendency to test all knowledge by its usefulness
in predicting and controlling the natural world.
The question of whether this emphasis might have contributed to a hidden
bias - that the scientific beliefs in reductionism (the idea that the best
and only way to understand something is to reduce it to its parts) and
positivism (the belief that only publicly measurable phenomena can be studied)
have prevented serious study of some aspects of human consciousness that
might be vital to everyone - ought not to be unaskable. Yet such
questions, potentially valuable as they might be, have not been seriously
pursued. In many instances, those who have attempted to do so were
actively discouraged by the scientific orthodoxy.
Since we can easily observe that other cultures, past and present,
have their hidden biases and blind spots - from societies that tolerate
slavery or cannibalism, to those that worship cats or live in fear of eclipses
- it is reasonable to assume that our cuulture probably has its own biases.
But to anyone with years of scientific training, all through which the
conviction was thoroughly drummed in that conventional science is our surest
guide to truth, it can come as a shock to realize that science itself (or
unreasoning belief in its unlimited power) might be seriously biased.
We have all chuckled over the story of the drunk who lost his house
key in a dark alley and was caught looking for it under the comer streetlight
"because the light is better here." Scientific research has been looking
largely "where the light is better'! - where the knowledge can be measured
and quantified, where hypotheses can be tested with neatly controlled,
repeatable experiments, where deterministic models and reductionistic explanations
seem to fit comfortably. As it happens, many useful things have been
learned under the light of science - but that doesn't mean that the world
outside that circle of light may not offer much of value, once it is illuminated
by an expanded concept of scientific inquiry.
The validity of science as a way of predicting and controlling many
aspects of the universe is not to be denied. However, as we shall
see later on, some of the most eminent scientists have begun to question
whether other kinds of knowledge less amenable to the prediction-and-control
criterion might be critically important at this time in history.
One of the more puzzling aspects of the history of science has been
its relative neglect of the topic of human consciousness - especially the
role of extraordinary states of consciousness in which creative breakthroughs
often occur. It is surely not because the topic was unimportant;
the study is quite evidently of central importance to any understanding
of human consciousness, motivation, and well-being. But it has been
an awkward subject to deal with.
Because of this extraordinary power to foster consensus, this "new
method of thought," as some European reformers referred to it when it was
first constructed, seemed to transcend all other ideologies. The
positivistic bias became an unspoken code of conduct for researchers, indicating
through the shared attitudes of a community what is "scientific" and what
is "not scientific." In this sense, "scientific" became a euphemism for
"proper."
Despite the universal consensus about scientific knowledge of the external
world, no such agreement has been reached about how to go about publicly
validating knowledge of private experience, in particular, the experience
of the deep intuition. Indeed, there is no obvious agreement about
the content of such knowledge, or its use to individuals or societies,
if the knowledge happens to be outside the image of human nature and capacities
that is maintained within industrial cultures. Strangely, this lack
of agreement among scientists differs sharply from the apparent recent
convergence of views held by various spiritual traditions around the world,
regarding the same subject.
The historical circumstance was such that positivism gained its power
before psychology and anthropology were developed, and before the influence
of unconscious beliefs was scientifically validated. The implications
that recent anthropological and psychological findings about beliefs and
belief systems contain, regarding the very nature of the scientific enterprise,
were not immediately and enthusiastically recognized, even in a community
committed to objectivity and the discovery of truth, for much the same
reasons that individuals resist self-knowledge that threatens their delusions.
The problem in dealing with such matters of the mind and the soul in
such a system is the difficulty in looking objectively at any minds or
souls other than one's own. These difficulties include the problem
of how to treat self - reports of subjective experience; the problem of
the relative non-replicability of consciousness-related phenomena; the
problem of observer influence; the problem of data reliability with sentient
subjects who are capable of conscious and unconscious choice deception;
the problem of individual uniqueness; the question of the place of purpose
and meaning; and the question of the appropriate basis for public validation
of that knowledge. The first scientists quite justifiably started
by looking at those aspects of nature that all observers could easily agree
upon.
Even before the solidification of the scientific method in the eighteenth
century and its marriage to industrial technology in the nineteenth century,
a worldwide consensus developed on how the positivistic kind of knowledge
should be openly sought and validated. Thus, in the physical and
biological sciences a coherent body of scientific knowledge developed,
with near global agreement about its contents. Because of this agreement,
we need not speak of an American biology or a German mathematics, a socialist
physics or a capitalist astronomy.
When science was young, important psychological and cultural factors
were combined in keeping the mysteries of human consciousness outside its
boundaries. Part of it was rooted in a desire to keep the scientific
mode of inquiry clearly distinct from the religious dogmatism of the past.
During its emergence in 17th century Europe, science encountered fierce
religious opposition, and the authority of the church was preeminent.
As a result, an almost territorial consensus developed, whereby science
would concern itself with the body and the physical world, and the church
would concern itself with the soul and the psyche.
But at the heart of the scientific hesitation to encompass subjective"
or "metaphysical" knowledge can be found not a methodological difficulty
or a scientific theory or a body of experimental findings, but a philosophical
attitude known as positivism, first articulated over a century ago.
This assumption places valid knowledge about reality exclusively in the
realm of direct, externally oriented, ("positive") sensory experience.
This unspoken, by now partly unconscious belief at the center of the scientific
system holds that only this kind of experience is verifiable, and hence
is the only proper base for valid ("scientific") knowledge.
The scientific method is of course not in any sense "wrong." Its present
form is simply not very suitable for probing some of the secrets of the
human mind, despite our widespread belief that the relentless progress
of science is marching toward solving all the mysteries of human nature.
The problem for those who have tried to study some of these mysteries has
been the unspoken, but widely implicit denial of the usefulness of looking
outside the illuminated circle of measurable, repeatable, predictable,
controllable data.
Since our readers, like ourselves, were raised within a society that
deeply cherishes the scientific tradition, many of you will undoubtedly
experience difficulty in accepting the suggestion that science as it exists
today might be perpetuating a limiting and damaging cultural bias.
We are not insisting that the bias be seen. Rather, we are raising
the question: "How do we know the bias is not present?" As the famous neurophysiologist
Warren McCulloch was known to say: "Don't bite my finger. Look where
I'm pointing."
Since the scientific method depends on logic, on the mathematical accumulation
of facts, and because this is how we have been taught to judge the reality
of an assertion, you would expect that the origin of the scientific method
itself would be the result of just such a rigorous compilation of cool,
dry, reason.
But strangely, there is a good case to be made for the proposition
that the scientific method was, to a significant degree, the result not
of logic, but of a fever dream, an experience of exactly the sort that
our cultural and scientific bias has taught us to shun. One of our
reasons for recounting this dream is to raise the question of what it might
imply if this is true.
THE MAN WHO DREAMED UP SCIENCE
It is rare indeed for historians to pinpoint a great cultural turning
point to a specific date and locale, and rarer still for them to agree
about the meaning of philosophical discoveries. But modern historians
concur that over three and a half centuries ago, in a day of thinking and
a night of dreaming, a twentythree-year-old soldier-philosopher succeeded
in reforming the entire structure of Western knowledge by setting down
the foundations of a new philosophy, science, mathematics - and a new way
of thinking about the world.
In 1619, in a form revealed by his visions and shaped by his labors,
Rene Descartes set down four rules for applying his new method for finding
truth:
1. Never to accept anything for true which I do not clearly know to be such.
2. Divide each of the difficulties under examination into as many parts as possible.
3. Begin with the simplest and easiest and then work step by step to the more complex.
4. Make enumerations so complete and reviews so general that I might be assured that nothing is omitted.
These rules and the entire Discourse on Method constituted a one-man watershed in the history of thought. Together with the thoughts and writings of other men in other countries at about the same time - Copernicus, Galileo, Francis Bacon among them - Descartes created the foundation of modern rationalism. The impact of Descartes' work on the world of his time was summarized by Jane Muir in Of Men and Numbers (Dodd Mead, 1966, p.47):
Truth lay just around the corner like a veiled statue waiting for men
to uncover it. The causes and effects of everything from comets to
heart palpitations could be made known, and, in Descartes' words, men would
soon be "the lords and possessors of nature. "
This was the epic scale on which Descartes dreamed. He had discovered
a wonderful new method for discerning the truth - a method totally different
from the ones men had been using. It was to apply the method of mathematics
to every area of life. By using logic, step by step, all the secrets
of nature could be laid bare. And with knowledge would come control.
Today, three hundred years later, when men can cause rain to fall,
plants to grow, or fatal diseases to disappear, the idea of mastering nature
or of understanding the universe from the molecule to the moon -
does not seem so new and wonderful. But three hundred years ago,
the idea that man might actually control nature was either unimaginable
or sacrilegious or both. It was arrogating God's power. Yet
Descartes, with a handful of other men - Francis Bacon being the most famous
- dared to think along this truly originnal line. And slowly, bit
by bit, man has made himself lord and possessor of nature.
Descartes was one of those rare cases where an entire civilization adopted
a new way of seeing the world at the instigation of one person. Others
added important refinements to his "method" - but the very idea of applying
a single, agreedupon method to the quest for truth was revolutionary.
Others took up the task of reforming the knowledge system of the day, turning
attention away from logics and theologies and toward direct observations
of nature. The grand unification of the sciences he predicted was
fulfilled beyond the original seer's wildest dreams - and as we shall see,
he went in for some wild dreams.
The success of this method, especially when it was turned to the ends
Francis Bacon urged - the control of nature, the "wresting of her secrets"
through the disciplined use of human minds - became the ideological framework
for science, and led to the marvelously effective technology used to build
the world that grew up around science. Taken together, the method,
the reformation, the unification, and the practical goal constitute what
has been called "the Cartesian worldview." In many ways, the actions of
the people who adopted this worldview constructed the world we live
in today.
Now that the problems of Western industrial society have become more
evident, and the unpleasant, possibly fatal sideeffects of controlling
nature by an uncontrolled science are becoming more widely known, the old
concept of "man conquering nature" has come under fire from some critics.
A few have gone to the extreme of denouncing scientific, analytic, "Cartesian"
thought as the primary cause of global problems. An inward but by
no means falsely modest person, Descartes himself would have been most
amused to learn that people would be debating the impact of his ideas three
and a half centuries
in the future.
Rene Descartes was born to a noble French family in 1596, and at the
age of ten commenced his studies of what was then the totality of Western
knowledge - logic, ethics, metaphysics, literature, history, science, and
mathematics. Since there had been few experimental scientists to
speak about since the fall of Rome, a great deal of this education had
to do with memorizing what the Greeks and Romans had to say. Descartes
wasn't entirely convinced that this was all there was to learn about the
world. In fact, at the age of eighteen, he declared that the whole
educational scheme was a farce, since the only certainty he had attained
in eight years of schooling was a knowledge of his own ignorance, and of
the limitations of the known systems of gathering and validating knowledge.
At that time, if a young French gentleman didn't study the classics,
he studied law, so Descartes studied law for two years in Poitiers.
As soon as he attained his degree, he declared law to be as intellectually
bankrupt as the rest of Western knowledge, and set off for Paris, where
he met notable
success - as a gambler.
Anyone who twice rejects the fruits of years of education, declaring
them inadequate to satisfy his need for knowledge, is a person who thinks
highly of his own intellectual abilities. Eventually, it dawned on
Descartes that if nobody else was going to think about making a little
progress beyond what Aristotle knew about things, it would have to be him.
Renouncing his decadent social life as abruptly as he had renounced
his earlier schooling, Descartes retired, at the age of twenty, to explore
the limits of knowledge on his own. He literally disappeared from
sight, leaving no forwarding address. His methodology, from
the beginning,
was unorthodox, and would have outraged many modern proponents of the scientific
method: It had long been his habit to linger half the day in bed, thinking
- an indulgence he continued through mucch of his life. Indeed, it
has been said that Descartes later invented analytic geometry while watching
a fly crawl on the ceiling.
After two years of this reclusive regimen, an old friend succeeded
in tracking down the reclining gentleman-philosopher, and persuaded
him to return to normal society, albeit briefly. The other young
aristocrats of his circle were hardly likely to appreciate the progress
he felt he was making in his thought-project. There was an amazing
lack of system to what his teachers had tried to pass off as knowledge,
he had been thinking. There were many explorations of the unknown
recorded in the books he read, and many little bits of knowledge scattered
sparsely through those books. But there was no method for gathering
and making sense of it all.
As was the custom for young gentlemen of his age and class, Descartes
volunteered for the army of the Prince of Nassau (confessing to a friend
that he was hardly aware of what side he was expected to fight on, and
who he was expected to fight) and was eventually ordered to Germany, where
the Thirty Years' War was underway. Although his external circumstances
had changed abruptly, his intellectual journey continued to carry him further
into unknown territory.
At that point in his life, Descartes was making monumental breakthroughs
in mathematics, and steady progress but no real breakthroughs in his search
for a new method of finding knowledge. Military service, apparently,
was no cause for suspending his intellectual endeavors. Quartered
in the town of Ulm for the winter, awaiting the resumption of hostilities
in the Spring, Descartes moved toward his moment of noetic thunder.
The night of November 10, 1619, found Descartes in an overheated room,
virtually feverish with "enthusiasm" about the intellectual adventure upon
which he had embarked. Descartes wrote much of his journal in Latin
(and it is worth remarking that the meaning of en theos - the root of enthusiasm
- is "God within"). That night he dreamed three dreams, sleep images
of such staggering import that he took care to write out detailed descriptions.
To the modem reader, the contents of the dreams may appear to be relatively
mundane, but Descartes was convinced that these enigmatic images held the
key to his quest for a new kind of knowledge.
In the first dream episode, he experienced strong winds blowing him
away from a church building toward a group of people who didn't appear
to be affected by the gale. After this image, Descartes awoke and
prayed for protection against the bad effects of the dream. Falling
asleep again, he was then filled with terror by a burst of noise like a
bolt of lightning, and dreaming that he was awake, saw a shower of sparks
fill his room. In the third and final dream of the series, Descartes
saw himself holding a dictionary and some papers, one of which contained
a poem beginning with the words "What path shall I follow in life?" An
unknown man handed him a fragment of verse - the words "Est et Non" caught
the dreamer's mind's eye.
At the end of the third dream came an even more extraordinary state
of consciousness - a dream within a dream. Descartes dreamed he awoke
to the fact that the shower of sparks in his room was in reality a dream,
and then he dreamed that he interpreted the previous dream!
In the dreamed interpretation, Descartes explained to himself that
the dictionary represented the future unity of science-" all the various
sciences grouped together"; the sheaf of poems symbolized the linkage of
philosophy and wisdom; "Est et Non" signified "Truth and Falsity in human
attainment and in secular sciences."
In his journals, Descartes revealed that he took the overall meaning
of the dreams to be that he was the person destined to reform knowledge
and unify the sciences, that the search for truth should be his career,
and that his thoughts of the previous months - about knowledge and methods
and a unifying system - were to become the foundation of a new method for
finding the truth.
Rene Descartes' diary entry for that day has since become famous.
It confidently proclaimed that he had constructed a universally applicable
tool for finding truth, and had answered his dissatisfaction with the previous
history of Western thought by building the foundations of "an admirable
science" which could proceed beyond the boundaries of existing methods
of inquiry.
"I begin to understand the foundations of a wonderful discovery ...
all the sciences are interconnected as by a chain; no one of them can be
completely grasped without taking in the whole encyclopaedia at once,"
he wrote, turning from his dream directly to the beginning of the work
that has come to us as The Discourse on Method, but which was originally
titled: "Project of a Universal Science Destined to Raise Our Nature to
Its Highest Degree of Perfection."
If that brilliant young aristocrat had not dreamed those three curious
dreams of November 10, 1619, the course of science and Western civilization
would have been significantly different. But on that winter evening
in Germany, during a lull between battles in a particularly vicious war,
the inner and outer spheres of knowledge met for a moment in the mind of
a particular human being. A surprisingly large portion of the world
we inhabit at this moment stems from that moment.
The dream of Descartes is an important episode in the secret history
of inspiration, for it represents a milestone in the institutionalization
of the taboo against inner knowing - and, ironically, also represents a
prime example of the potential power of the same inner knowledge.
Although the ideas that manifested in that moment appear to have been apprehended
in an altered state of consciousness, and involved non-rational means of
"knowing," a direct consequence of the revelation was the growth of rational,
reductionist, positivist science the same institution that later
questioned the validity of "subjective knowledge."
There has been no argument, even from the most conservative historians,
about the fact that Descartes' dreams did occur on the same day as his
epochal journal entries. His own account attributes the inspiration
for his "admirable science" to these dreams. In fact, Descartes stated
that the dream was the most decisive event in his life, and proclaimed
his intention to make a pilgrimage from Venice to Lorette as an act of
thanksgiving for this supernatural guidance.
Upon reflection, it is odd that the dream-origins of modern thought
are but a footnote to history. While Descartes' more useful ideas
were adopted wholeheartedly, the reaction to their source was savagely
negative. Descartes himself noted the deep importance of his dream
images as well as his calculations and logical operations as tools for
constructing the method. But few of his contemporaries would accept
the anomaly of the dream-knowledge.
When Baillet's Life of M. Descartes was first published, August Comte
(the originator of the philosophy of positivism) referred to that fateful
night as a "cerebral episode." Christian Huygens, a towering scientific
figure of the day, at the urging of the equally monumental Gottfried Leibniz,
wrote: "The passage in which he relates how his brain was overstimulated
and in a fit state for visions, and his vow to Our Lady of Lorette, shows
great weakness...." Baillet himself tried to reduce the embarrassing illumination
to a case of nervous exhaustion: "He tired himself to such an extent that
his brain became overheated and he fell into a kind of rapture which
so worked upon his already exhausted spirit that it became predisposed
to the reception of dreams and visions."
Suffice it to note that Descartes engaged in a long, intense period
of preparatory thought before his night of revelation, that the insights
came in a series of images, one of which was the image of a bolt of lightning,
and the message not only addressed broad, abstract topics like the search
for truth, but directly addressed the question of what the dreamer should
do with his life.
One reason we have dwelt on this tale is to draw attention to the non-rational
moments at the very foundations of science. But the fact that the
new method of thought Descartes dreamed up evolved into a belief system
known as contemporary science is also important to our discussion.
Science is not a static edifice, built out of facts piled up like so many
bricks, but is in fact a way of looking at the world, a perspective that
changes from time to time. This fact was not a popular thought in
science until very recently.
The psychological aspects of the process of science only became a subject
of critical inquiry after Thomas Kuhn's 1962 publication of The Structure
of Scientific Revolutions. In that book, Kuhn challenged the then
dominant view that scientific progress is a simple cumulative process.
He did this by introducing the notion of "paradigm." In the sense that
Kuhn meant it, paradigms are the systems of common assumptions that scientists
(and others) share about the meaning of problems they consider. To
a scientist, or a citizen of a society, a "dominant paradigm" might not
even be consciously articulated, but it is nonetheless a basic way of perceiving,
thinking, valuing, and doing, associated with a particular vision of reality.
A dominant paradigm is seldom if ever stated explicitly; it exists
as an unquestioned, tacit understanding that is transmitted through the
culture and to succeeding generations through direct experience (rather
than being "taught"). A paradigm cannot be defined precisely in a
few well-chosen sentences. In fact, it is not something to be expressed
verbally at all. It is what the anthropologist hopes to understand
after he or she has lived in a foreign culture for a long time - what the
natives in a society perceive with their eyes and value with their feelings.
A dominant paradigm encompasses more than an ideology or a world view but
less than a total culture.
Other social scientists and historians such as Lewis Mumford, Arnold
Toynbee, and Fred Polak, whose ideas will be discussed in other chapters,
have pointed out the ways in which civilization-wide paradigms sometimes
undergo rapid, violent transformations. The breakdown of the industrial-era
paradigm and its replacement by another would be accompanied by drastic
changes in our society's operative values and institutions. More
profoundly, it would involve a change in the basic perception of reality.
We recall our definition of the social paradigm as "the basic way of
perceiving, thinking, valuing, and doing, associated with a particular
vision of reality." It is change in that vision of reality that is the
hallmark of the historically rare fundamental transformations of civilizations.
We have noted a close relationship between three kinds of paradigm-shifts
that appear to be in the process of occurring: First, there is a change
of paradigm in the sciences involved with the study of human nature, a
shift to a way of seeing the world that no longer excludes those profound
experiences that are difficult to quantify but which seem to be the source
of our most deeply held values. Second is the paradigm shift within
individuals - the breakthrough experience that often precedes and
accompanies the attainment of higher knowledge and capabilities.
Last is the most important transformation, the system-wide transformation
of our society, which we think is closely related to the other two varieties
of paradigm-shift.