Nonetheless, had the electorate not been convinced that the election was over even before the day to vote had arrived, the increased turn-out which would have occurred would have given him the votes he needed in the key states where the election was decided to have won. XIII. Pretenses of Objectivity There can be a number of difficulties with the polling process, but, with or without conscious bias, these surveys can err in terms of their queries or samples. Often, while a determination is made as to the leanings of those interviewed, such information is generally glossed over in the reporting of results. A likely voter indicating they would probably vote for Clinton were the election held that day might be identified or identifiable as someone 'leaning' that way merely, or differentiated from more 'solid' Clinton support because they happen to be Republican or independent and leaning toward such a vote. Such characteristics could be crucial in understanding the potential vote options which might occur. Hypothetically, a survey reporting a 51% support for Clinton, within a margin of error of four points, may actually be a report of 40% solidly for him, and 11% leaning that way. It might also consist of 33 % Democrats leaning toward Clinton, with the other portion actually being 12% independents and 6% Republicans likely to support him. In either event, the glossing over of such information does little to add credibility to the poll and may reflect a partisan predisposition by the pollster, the analyst, or the reporter. Sampling can be just as precarious an exercise. In order to have any chance of accuracy whatsoever, the sampling must be not only random and scientific, but scientifically random. Not only would problems with selection skew the results, but the method of aggregation can be a difficulty. Since over recent elections, black voters have cast ballots at the level of 95% for Democrats seeking the White House, such calculation may be built into the poll, assuming such numbers. Where the black vote changed, as it did in 1996, to a 'mere' 85% Democrat, the impact on the numbers would be huge. If there were twenty million black voters, such a change would reflect a shift of some two million votes into the Republican column, enough to significantly alter the election. Even if we give pollsters the benefit of the doubt, something increasingly difficult to do, that cannot be done for the utilization of such survey data by the media. What gets reported, when and how it is structured, the words or tone or even body language used, all will impact on what the person hearing the report thinks. But this is a problem beyond polling reports. Opponents of SDI always call it Star Wars, and reports on the economy in 1992 routinely described only 4% growth in GDP, while in 1996, reports had the economy 'surging' ahead at an annual GDP growth rate of 3%. Part of the problem is that the media present themselves under the guise of objectivity(Bennet 1988). Not only are they not objective, but they cannot be, nor do they seem to make much effort to be. And for all of these reasons, polls are not objective, either. And yet, we are constantly bombarded with such 'objective, factual' reporting. Even for a cynical public, there is something about numbers which may lead many to conclude their reliability, especially when the patter that was consistently fed to the public during the summer and fall of 1996 was of the character it was. That does not connote any 'conspiracy' of pollsters or anyone else to 'program' the electorate, but it does explain both polling inaccuracy in this election as well as others, as well as the public's apparent acquiescence in the 'inevitable' Dole loss. Compounded by the level of misinformation, the results should hardly have been expected to have been much different than they turned out. XIV. Pavlov's Voters? The Perot phenomenon itself was, while not exclusively, in large measure a media- conjured event. In spite of his millions, had the press not trumped him up, had they been as keen in scrutinizing him as they have been on some other matters, and should have been regarding him, he would never have 'happened' at all, either in 1992 or 1996. In fact, the refusal of the media to play him the same in 1996 as they had in the previous election probably had as much to do with his lower polling and vote numbers as anything else. But then, in 1996, there was a certain 'fear' that Perot might hurt Clinton more than he would whomever the GOP ran. We had a similar phenomenon in 1996. The media was a major factor in the strength of Buchanan, as it had been for him in 1992. The evaluation of primaries and other delegate selection processes is sometimes curious. Often, a candidate coming in second (or even lower) is the 'winner' because they exceeded the commentator's expectations. The spotlight on Buchanan did some harm to the Dole campaign also because Dole had to spend money to win the nomination, and was left with no resources to counteract the Clinton/Democrat/union blitz that ran throughout the spring and summer nationally and locally around the country. For some considerable period of time, the Dole effort was hamstrung during the primary process because of spending constraints. And for a brief moment, the media seemed to be going to do the same thing with Steve Forbes that they had with Buchanan. Perhaps, though, he and/or his ideas became too viable, because the 'romance' ended quickly in scurrilous reporting, although the effort did have some negative impact on the Dole campaign effort. But undoubtedly the most insidious tactic used against Bob Dole was the matter of his age. It was heralded as an issue, but then downplayed, though frequently referentially implied in word and picture. Any such suggestion should have infuriated senior citizens even more than the rest of us, but so, too, should have Clinton's tax on social security benefits, instead of the fear-mongering campaign about nonexistent Republican attempts to 'slash' Medicare and Social Security and let old people die in the streets. It is a tremendous handicap to have to overcome such levels of disinformation, either for a Republican candidate or for a voter, for whom the costs of information can be staggering and stifling. The pollsters reported such findings, too. Republicans were seen as mean-spirited, as having slashed spending, etc., by the public. Where did they got that idea? A better analogy than that of an electorate 'programmed' or 'conditioned,' although there is some validity to such notions if only as the matter of information costs, might be the spirit engendered in the citizenry by the surveys and their endless presentation. That was more the reason for the decline in voting turn-out that the much touted antipathy for politics due to concern over its control by monied special interests. It is not as disparaging of the American voters to suggest that they might have been 'set up' to believe Dole could not beat Clinton as it is to engage in any such effort, whether by plan, concerted effort, or more likely on partisan bias of those in a position to pull it off. This does not require conspiracy. It is part of the liberal mind-set that they know better than the rest of us what we think, do, and say. But in spite of the spirit conjured up by the politicos, in large measure built on the statistical data of pollsters, the best efforts of the pollster geist could only narrowly win over Bush and Dole, and could not turn out the House. XV. The Media-tion of George Bush It is old wisdom that Presidential approval ratings tend to track in an inverse relationship to the rate of unemployment. If unemployment rises, approval will decline and vice versa. In the case of George Bush, there would seem to be a firm basis for such a conclusion. As unemployment rates varied through 1991 and 1992, Bush's rating varied just as would be anticipated.

Table XI -- Presidential Approval and Unemployment Rate (Public Perspective 1990-92) Month Unemployment Rate Approval Rate 9/91 6.7 71 10/91 6.8 65 11/91 6.8 51 12/91 7.1 51 1/92 7.2 48 2/92 7.3 42--2;40 3/92 7.3 41 4/92 7.2 39 5/92 7.5 35 6/92 7.8 34 7/92 7.7 37-->31 8/92 7.6 38 9/92 7.5 42-->40

It is not sufficient, however, to conclude causality from coincidence, although a strong enough correlation does present considerable grounds. But there are obviously other factors which can contribute to approval rate vacillations. While it is difficult to argue absolute causality from these, it certainly creates an appearance that there may be some causal connection arguable, and the occurrence of certain events at points of vacillation in approval rating provides at least some food for thought, as for example, Bush ratings sky- rocketing to near 90% during Desert Storm. It is also notable that some of the variation in Bush approval rating apparently took place irrespective of changes in the unemployment rate. Continue 1